Skip to main content

Full text of "Uses of the Thematic Apperception Test"

See other formats


USES OF THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST * 
HENRY A. MURRAY, M.D., Pu. D., Camsprince, Mass. 


The choice of “projective techniques” as 
topic for the theoretical section at the 1950 
meeting of The American Psychiatric Asso- 
ciation is another heartening sign, an au- 
thoritative sign, of multiplying articulations 
of interest and purpose between practitioners 
of psychiatry and practitioners of psychol- 
ogy. It seems that the older and more vener- 
able of the two professions is today both 
secure enough and magnanimous enough to 
give the bumptious younger one an oppor- 
tunity to speak up and be heard. 

The choice of this topic also indicates, it 
seems to me, a mounting enthusiasm among 
psychiatrists for investigations of a strictly 
psychological sort, in addition to the ever- 
important researches of a physiological sort. 

I would be not a little embarrassed to de- 
vote, as I will now, all the allotted time to 
a test with which my name is sometimes 
linked, if this test were not a product of 
more brains than mine. The germinal sug- 
gestion for the TAT came from a brilliant 
student in abnormal psychology at Rad- 
cliffe, Mrs. Cecilia Roberts,? and, during 
the first phases of its development, much of 
the picture selection and picture drawing, the 
administration and interpretation, was done 
by Mrs. Christiana D. Morgan. Since then 
a host of ladies and gentlemen—Drs. White, 
Sanford, Tomkins, Bellak, Henry, Rapa- 
port, Stein, Rosenzweig, and others—have 
succeeded more than I have in shaping its 
character. 

This afternoon, with your tolerance, I 
shall assume the role of protagonist and, to 
sharpen the argument, put forth the pre- 
posterous proposition that the psychiatrist 
himself—particularly the psychoanalytically 
trained psychiatrist—should learn the simple 
art of administering and interpreting the 
TAT. 

My first reason for suggesting this is 
tinged with selfish prejudice. Having a cer- 


1 Read at the 106th annual meeting of The Ameri- 
can Psychiatric Association, Detroit, Mich., May 


1-5, 1950. 
2 Now Mrs. Crane Brinton. 


2 


tain sentimental regard for the TAT, I am 
anxious that the young lady be given every 
opportunity for refinement and for the ex- 
hibition eventually of all her potential charms 
and talents. Without the aid of the psycho- 
analyst, this goal is scarcely attainable, be- 
cause not only is the analyst in the best rela- 
tional position to kindle the patient’s whole 
capacity for projective story-telling, but with 
the knowledge he acquires from free associa- 
tions and dream analyses, he, more than 
anyone, is capable of discriminating grain 
from chaff in the TAT stories, and thus of 
laying hold on the facts required for the 
construction of dependable principles of 
interpretation. 

Whatever peculiar virtue the TAT may 
have, if any, it will be found to reside, not, 
as some have assumed, in its power to mirror 
overt behavior or to communicate what the 
patient knows and is willing to tell, but 
rather in its capacity to reveal things that the 
patient is unwilling to tell or is unable to 
tell because he is unconscious of them. Since 
it is only the depth therapist who, in the 
regular course of his work, exposes compo- 
nents of the personality that have been un- 
conscious to the patient, it is only the depth 
psychotherapist who is in a position to 
validate the most significant inferences 
drawn from the TAT stories. Thus, fur- 
ther straight-line progress in the develop- 
ment of this technique depends to a con- 
siderable extent on whether or not a few 
competent psychiatrists will decide that 
the TAT is a strategic instrument for ex- 
plorations of subterranean mental processes. 

More specifically and more cogently, I 
would recommend the use of this device at 
the start, in the middle, and at the end of 
courses of therapy, first of all as an aid in 
identifying suppressed and repressed dis- 
positions and conflicts, and in defining, as 
Bellak(1) has suggested, the nature of the 
patient’s resistances to these dispositions ; 
second as a therapeutic agent, since the 
stories, like dreams, provide admirable start- 
ing points for free associations; third as a 


577 


578 


means of estimating the effects of therapy; 
and fourth as an instrument of research, espe- 
cially in the psychosomatic disorders. 

Administration—Although the TAT is 
rarely administered as I believe it should be, 
the technique is very simple, if you happen 
to be the kind of person who is disposed to 
hearten people in their creative efforts. All 
you have to do is to recite a short paragraph 
of plain instructions and with an encouraging 
expression—I won’t say a grin—hand the 
patient Picture No. 1. 

In order to prevent the much too common 
occurrence of more or less irrelevant reac- 
tions—such as mere descriptions of parts 
of the picture—we at the Harvard Clinic 
have adopted the practice of requesting the 
patient to examine the picture carefully for 
about 20 seconds and then to put it aside. 

Also, in order to facilitate the establish- 
ment in the patient of a single individual 
point of orientation through identification 
with a preferred figure, we ask him or her 
to choose a proper name for the chief charac- 
ter before proceeding with the story. 

It is often necessary, after the completion 
of the first story, to repeat some of the di- 
rections, explaining unambiguously to the 
patient that every story he tells must have 
a plot with a definite ending. But after 
this—except for an occasional guiding com- 
ment and some judicious praise—the ad- 
ministrator should not say anything until 
10 stories have been told and the hour is 
over. 

If properly “warmed up,” most subjects 
(instructed to devote about 5 minutes to 
each response) will tell stories that are 200 
words or more in length (as recorded, say, 
on a dictaphone). Although there are cer- 
tainly some psychotics and an occasional 
neurotic who cannot be induced under ordi- 
nary circumstances—say, without the ad- 
ministration of a drug—to tell stories of 
this length, or even to speak at all, we con- 
sider that to come out with stories averaging 
less than 200 words apiece usually indicates 
that the rapport between administrator and 
the patient and/or the “warming up” process 
were defective. 

At the moment we are testing the effec- 
tiveness of other directions given to the 
testee. Instead of asking for one long story, 


USES OF THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 


[ Feb. 


we request the subject to respond to each 
picture by presenting the outline of as many 
plots as possible. Although something is 
lost by this method, something is gained: 
we obtain about 70 themes instead of 20. 
It is too early to say whether, on the average, 
the gains outweigh the losses. 

Test Material—Physically speaking, as 
some of you may know, the TAT is no more 
nor less than a set of 19 pictures and one 
blank card arranged in a definite order. 

The advantages of keeping the stimulus 
conditions of a test uniform—of presenting, 
say, an unchanging set of pictures in an 
unchanging sequence—are generally known 
and appreciated. No argument for this prin- 
ciple seems necessary today, despite the 
fact that the majority of TAT workers, as 
far as I can determine, have not seen fit to 
abide by it. 

Unless we accept this amount of stand- 
ardization it will not be possible to do what 
we so often want to do: compare the re- 
sponses of one subject, or of one class of 
subjects, or of one social group, with the 
responses of other subjects, classes, social 
groups. Every TAT worker knows that 
the kinds of responses—in this case, stories— 
that he gets are largely determined by the 
characteristics of the pictures. In order to 
raise the proportion, say, of homicidal and 
suicidal themes, one has only to introduce 
one new element in one picture—a gun lean- 
ing against a wall. 

In view of these weighty considerations, 
we TAT workers might be disposed to stick 
to the standard set of pictures, were it not 
that so many of us believe that some of these 
pictures are not as significantly provocative 
as they might be. We can hardly doubt, for 
example, that Thompson(4) is correct in 
stating that color increases the stimulating 
power of the pictures. The introduction of 
two or three abstract or symbolic pictures— 
less definite, less structured—might also im- 
prove the series. 

It is not unlikely that the deeper layers of 
fantasy would be more successfully invited 
by pictures that were less closely related to 
settings and personages of everyday Ameri- 
can life. A foreign landscape, a fairy tale 
scene, or an animal picture might arouse 
fewer defenses than do some of the pictures 


1951] 


now in use. Furthermore, as Shakow ® and 
others have pointed out, certain often-critical 
conditions—such as sibling rivalry, sepa- 
ration from a supporting person, and so 
forth—are not suggested by any of the pic- 
tures in the present collection. Finally, in 
order to avoid antagonizing subjects who 
have esthetic sensibilities, it is evident that 
several of the Harvard pictures must be re- 
drawn and all of them should be more satis- 
fyingly reproduced. 

Thus, we have two conflicting aims: One, 
to establish and agree to use a standard set 
of pictures, and two, to improve the present 
set. These aims, as I see them, can be 
reconciled only by delegating to an elected 
committee the responsibility of judging 
whether or not each new picture submitted 
for membership in the series is more effec- 
tive than the least effective picture in cur- 
rent use. 

In terms of what criteria should these 
judgments be made? In my opinion, the most 
readily obtainable criteria—length, vividness, 
and dramatic intensity of the stories—cri- 
teria proposed by Symonds(3), Thompson 
(4), and others are not at all dependable. 
What we really need to know is how much 
each picture commonly contributes to an un- 
derstanding of the patient’s latent, repressed, 
and unconscious dispositions. Since the TAT 
is not designed to exhibit the overt action- 
patterns of people, the possession by any 
picture of this kind of virtuosity is almost 
wholly irrelevant. If the TAT selection 
committee agrees with this opinion, the data 
they will require in appraising the effective- 
ness of any picture can be obtained only 
through an extensive study of the covert 
personalities of a large number of subjects 
who have taken the test. 

Besides an improved set of 20 or 30 
pictures for general use, I would strongly 
recommend several special sets, of 4 or 5 
pictures each, for testing the presence of 
specific dispositions or complexes. 

Constituents of TAT Stories—The ef- 
ficacy of the TAT, like that of most projec- 
tion tests, depends on the degree to which 
the following assumptions are valid: 

1. In characterizing the hero of a story 
and in portraying his actions and reactions, 


8 Personal communication. 


HENRY A. MURRAY 


9/9 


the storyteller will commonly utilize some 
of the components, conscious or unconscious, 
of his own past or present personality—for 
example, an assumption, an expectation, an 
idea, a feeling, an evaluation, a need, a 
plan, or a fantasy that he has experienced 
or entertained. 

2. In characterizing the other major fig- 
ures of a dramatic narrative and in por- 
traying their actions and reactions, the story- 
teller will commonly utilize some of the 
personality components (as he has apper- 
ceived them) of persons—such as parents, 
siblings, rivals, loved objects—with whom he 
has had, or is having, significant interactions. 

Not infrequently some of the depicted 
qualities and reactions of the other major 
characters will be derived from once-fan- 
tasied figures—inventions of the child’s im- 
agination—rather than from apperceptions 
of actual people; or they may be derived 
from aspects of the storyteller’s own person- 
ality (as in the first assumption). That is to 
say, the interactions in a story may involve 2 
different parts—two subsystems—of the sub- 
ject’s total self. 

3. In constructing the plot, describing the 
endeavors of the hero, his transactions with 
the other major figures, and the outcome 
and final consequences of these efforts and 
interactions, the storyteller will commonly 
utilize memory traces, conscious or uncon- 
scious, of some of the actual or fantasied 
events that have exerted a significant in- 
fluence on his development. 

Note that these are not only very modest 
assumptions, assumptions that have been 
made by generations of literary critics, but 
that they have been repeatedly demonstrated. 

According to the 3 stated propositions, only 
a fraction—as a rule a relatively small frac- 
tion—of the aggregate of words, phrases, 
and sentences that make up a set of stories 
represent important constituents (as defined 
above) of the patient’s past or present per- 
sonality. Asa rule, most of the obtained ma- 
terial consists of statements that are not 
representative of anything that needs to be 
included in a formulation of his personality. 
In short, the larger fraction of the protocol 
is chaff; the smaller fraction, grain. The 
crucial question—how does one thresh out 
the grain from these stalks of stories—will 
be discussed shortly. 


380 


The assumption that a set of TAT stories 
will contain a fair amount of grain—some- 
times a large amount of rich grain—cannot 
be verified by observing the subject’s be- 
havior in everyday life. The patterns of 
the imagination and the patterns of public 
conduct are more apt to be related by con- 
trast than by conformity. But the psychia- 
trist can prove to himself and others how 
much real grain is concealed in TAT stories 
by waiting a few months until he has ac- 
quired a great deal of information and feels 
thoroughly at home in his patient’s stream 
of consciousness. If then, at this later date, 
he examines the set of stories, with mind 
alert to every symbolic possibility, he will 
almost surely discover that a good deal of 
what he has learned during the course of 
the analysis is there, varyingly disguised, in 
the stories. 

Of course some important things, as Tom- 
kins(5) and Bellak(1) have pointed out, 
will not be found there. Two hours of story- 
telling is not enough to reveal all the im- 
portant potentialities of a person, and be- 
sides this, the ego has its defenses that 
operate even when self-consciousness is half- 
lost in the process of composing dramatic 
plots. 

If, say, in the middle of an analysis, the 
psychiatrist uses the critical elements and 
incidents in the stories as points of departure 
for free associations, and by tactful ques- 
tioning discovers the known sources of as 
many items as possible, and then adds this 
information to the knowledge he has al- 
ready acquired, he will usually find that the 
grain—that is, the significant personal refer- 
ences in the stories—can be assigned to one 
or more of the following periods of the life 
history. 

1. Testing Period—The TAT protocol 
is likely to include some indication of the 
patient’s apperception, appraisal, and reac- 
tion to the total testing situation and/or, 
more specifically, the administrator of the 
test. 

2. Current Period—Many of the TAT 
grains are straight or distorted representa- 
tions of constituents of what historians call 
the “specious present.” That is, they portray 
the patient’s evaluations, emotional reac- 
tions, and expectations in relation to the 


USES OF THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 


[ Feb. 


on-going course of events, the events that 
in the last days, weeks, or months have most 
frequently or intensely affected him. 

3. Past Periods——Of these the period of 
childhood is perhaps most important from a 
therapeutic standpoint. 

According to our experience almost all 
TAT protocols contain grains that can be 
interpreted as symbolic representations of 
childhood occurrences. I would be sur- 
prised if there is any traumatic event or 
complex known to child psychology that 
has not been found in some disguised form 
in TAT protocols. 

So much for the constituents of TAT 
stories and the periods of the life history 
from which they are commonly derived. 

Let us now turn to the as-yet-unsolved, 
or only partly solved, problem of how to pick 
the significant elements and forms out of a 
web of irrelevancies, when one’s knowledge 
of the patient’s past history and character 
is nil. 

Here I must be brief. Time is running out. 
The most dependable criteria, I submit, for 
distinguishing the relevant elements and 
forms in a set of stories are the following: 

1. Symbolic significance: 1.e., an element 
or thematic structure that resembles in some 
familiar way an element or theme that is 
known to be very commonly important in 
childhood. Here I am referring to plausible 
inferences based on our knowledge of the 
principles of dream interpretation. 

2. Repetition: 1.e., an element or theme 
that recurs 3 or more times in the series of 
stories. 

3. Uniqueness: by referring to the set 
of norms recently published by Rosenzweig 
(2) the inexperienced interpreter can, for 
the first time, make use of this criterion. 

4. Interrelatedness: 1.€., an element or 
theme that is known to be mutually related 
with an element or theme that has already 
been judged to be significant (according to 
one of the 3 above-listed criteria.) 

5. Subject’s self-involvement: 1.e., indica- 
tions that the subject’s emotions—interests 
or defenses—were excited when mentioning 
a certain element, or during the entire com- 
position or part of it. 

So much for the huge subject of diag- 
nosis. 


1951] 


This brings me to the end of my allotted 
time and the end of the most condensed 
summary I could contrive of the Thematic 
Apperception Test and its uses. 


BIBLIOGRAPHY 
1. Bellak, L. Thematic apperception: Failures 
and the defenses. Trans. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 12: 122, 
1950. 
2. Rosenzweig, S. Apperceptive norms for the 


HENRY A. 


MURRAY 581 


Thematic Apperception Test. J. Personal., 17: 475, 
1949. 

3. Symonds, P. Adolescent Fantasy. New York, 
Columbia University Press, 1949. 

4. Thompson, C. E., and Bachrach, A. J. The 
use of color in the Thematic Apperception Test. 
Presented at the Annual Meeting of the AAAS, 
New York, 1949. 

5. Tomkins, S. S. The present status of the 
Thematic Apperception Test. Am. J. Ortho- 
psychiat., 19: 358, 1949.